Adverse psychological outcomes in colorectal cancer screening: Does health anxiety play a role?

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2005.08.011Get rights and content

Abstract

People who are anxious about their health are more likely to misinterpret health information as personally threatening and less likely to be reassured by medical investigations that show they are free from disease. Consequently, health anxious people would be expected to react more adversely to cancer screening, but this possibility has rarely been explored. The moderating role of health anxiety on the psychological impact of participating in colorectal cancer screening was examined among a sub-sample of 3535 participants in a large, community-based trial of colorectal cancer screening in the UK. The screening modality was flexible sigmoidoscopy, which examines the bowel for pre-cancerous polyps. It was predicted that health anxiety would be associated with more worry about cancer before screening, a greater increase in worry if polyps were detected, and less reassurance after a clear result. As expected, health anxious participants were more anxious and more worried about bowel cancer both before and after screening. However, they experienced greater reductions in anxiety and worry about cancer following the examination. They reported lower levels of reassurance following screening, but also expressed more positive reactions to the experience. The positive psychological benefits of attending medical investigations should be examined in future work, because this may go some way towards explaining why health anxious people repeatedly seek medical interventions.

Introduction

Individual differences in the way people process health-related information can influence its emotional impact. For example, people with a ‘monitoring’ attentional style are thought more likely to amplify health threats, to believe they are at risk of illness and to respond with greater levels of distress (Miller, Fang, Manne, Engstrom, & Daly, 1999). Such individual differences may in turn be modified by factors such as personal goals (Carver & Scheier, 1998), dispositional optimism (Andrykowski et al., 2002; Miller, Shoda, & Hurley, 1996), and current mood (Salovey & Birnbaum, 1989). One of the most serious health threats people face is cancer. It is the second most common cause of premature death after cardiovascular disease, and nationwide screening programmes mean that everyone is invited to be screened. Cancer screening therefore has the potential to cause an increase in anxiety about cancer.

Early detection tests, such as mammography screening for breast cancer and faecal occult blood (FOB) screening for colorectal cancer, can diagnose cancer, but they also yield a proportion of false positive results, where an apparently abnormal finding requires further investigation before cancer can be ruled out. The experience of a ‘near-miss’ may affect people's sense of invulnerability to illness or induce a pre-occupation with hidden disease. Tests aiming to detect pre-cancerous changes, rather than cancer per se (e.g. Pap smears for cervical cancer), identify a proportion of individuals as ‘high risk’ because they have abnormalities that could turn into cancer if left untreated (Bell et al., 1995; Ellman et al., 1989; Gilbert et al., 1998; Gram, Lund, & Slenker, 1990; Schwartz, Woloshin, Fowler, & Welch, 2004). This outcome may lead to distress at the realisation that an abnormality has been developing without the individual's knowledge and concerns that the problem may recur.

The number of people diagnosed with cancer at screening is relatively low, but a substantial proportion of people attending screening will experience false positives or be found to have pre-cancerous abnormalities (Hofvind, Thoresen, & Tretli, 2004). The psychological distress arising from both false positive and high risk results has been shown to be relatively short-lived (Ellman et al., 1989; Lampic, Thurfjell, Bergh, & Sjoden, 2001; Lowe, Balanda, Del Mar, & Hawes, 1999; Parker, Robinson, Scholefield, & Hardcastle, 2002; Rimer & Bluman, 1997; Sutton, Saidi, Bickler, & Hunter, 1995; Thiis-Evensen, Wilhelmsen, Hoff, Blomhoff, & Sauar, 1999; Wardle, Pernet, & Stephens, 1995; Wardle et al., 2003), however lingering concerns about the possible psychological sequelae of screening remain. A number of studies have shown that cancer-specific worries may persist for months and even years after screening (Absetz, Aro, & Sutton, 2003; Aro, Pilvikki Absetz, van Elderen, van der Ploeg, & van der Kamp, 2000; Brett & Austoker, 2001; Gram et al., 1990; Idestrom, Milsom, & Andersson-Ellstrom, 2003; Lowe et al., 1999). There is also evidence of vulnerable subgroups for whom a false positive or high risk result is particularly worrying. Neuroticism (Chen et al., 1996), poor social support networks, and information-seeking (‘monitoring’) coping styles (Clutton, Pakenham, & Buckley, 1999) have all been found to increase the impact of abnormal screening results. Surprisingly, the role of health anxiety in exacerbating any adverse emotional effects of screening has rarely been explored.

Health anxiety is usually thought of as a continuum with ‘hypochondriasis’ at the extreme (Salkovskis & Bass, 1997; Warwick & Salkovskis, 1990). It is characterised by attentional biases towards illness-related information (Owens, Asmundson, Hadjistavropoulos, & Owens, 2004), and cognitive biases leading to the misinterpretation of information as personally threatening and catastrophic. Health anxious individuals are more likely to believe that somatic sensations signal serious illness (MacLeod, Haynes, & Sensky, 1998). They visit their doctor more frequently (Barsky, Ettner, Horsky, & Bates, 2001) and believe that their symptoms warrant referral to a specialist (Conroy, Smyth, Siriwardena, & Fernandes, 1999). They also show adverse emotional reactions to ambiguous diagnostic test results (Rimes & Salkovskis, 2002) and are less reassured following the medical investigation of symptoms, even when there is no evidence of disease (Lucock, White, Peake, & Morley, 1998; Meechan, Collins, Moss-Morris, & Petrie, 2005).

On the basis of the findings noted above, health anxious individuals would be expected to react more adversely to cancer screening. In addition, such adverse reactions may be particularly likely following screening for pre-malignant conditions (e.g. cervical smears) because of the greater potential for misunderstanding associated with such tests (Kavanagh & Broom, 1997).

The present study assessed the role of health anxiety in moderating the psychological impact of flexible sigmoidoscopy (FS) screening for the prevention of colorectal cancer, as part of the UK FS trial. Unlike previous research, this is a prospective study, allowing us to assess change in anxiety as a consequence of participating in a cancer screening procedure.

FS screening is designed to detect pre-malignant growths (adenomatous polyps) in the bowel and remove them before they become malignant. In the UK FS Trial, screening outcomes were categorised into three groups (those diagnosed with cancer were followed up separately): (i) negative result: no polyps detected, (ii) lower risk result: polyps were detected that were not classified as higher risk and were removed during the examination, and (iii) higher risk result: any of the following were detected—polyps above 1 cm in size; three or more adenomatous polyps; polyps with tubulovillous or villous histology, or severe dysplasia; or 20 or more hyperplastic polyps. People classified as higher risk were referred for a colonoscopy—a more invasive examination which inspects the whole of the bowel, during which any polyps were removed. A small number of cancers were diagnosed, but these cases are not included in the present analysis because they were followed-up separately (Miles, Wardle, & Atkin, 2003).

It was predicted that: (1) health anxious participants would be more worried about bowel cancer prior to attending screening, (2) health anxiety would be associated with more worry and poorer psychological well-being following a higher risk screening result, and (3) health anxiety would be associated with less reassurance and greater cancer worry following a negative screening outcome, especially among those who reported bowel symptoms before coming in for the screening examination.

Section snippets

Design

In total, 4329 people attended for FS screening in the three trial centres where the emotional consequences of screening were investigated (Oxford, Portsmouth and Swansea) (see Atkin et al. (2001) for full details of the trial design). A prospective design was used, with participants being sent postal questionnaires 6 months before, and three months after, their screening appointment. Health anxiety and bowel symptoms were assessed before screening, to test whether they moderated the impact of

Response rate

In total, 4329 people attended screening. 2865 (66.2%) had negative results, 1198 (27.7%) had lower risk findings, 248 (5.7%) had higher risk findings and 18 were diagnosed with cancer (0.4%). In total, 3535 (81.6%) completed both the pre- and post-screening questionnaires (‘completers’) and these are the sample for analysis. Fig. 1 shows the study attrition. To assess whether completers differed from non-completers, the two groups were compared in terms of age, sex and screening outcome; the

Discussion

This study on the effects of health anxiety is one of the largest to be carried out in a real-life context and has the advantage of a prospective design. Consistent with our predictions, health anxious participants were both more anxious and more worried about bowel cancer prior to screening than non-health anxious participants. However, mixed support was found for the hypothesis that health anxious participants would respond more adversely to screening. People high on health anxiety

References (47)

  • S.M. Miller et al.

    Decision making about prophylactic oophorectomy among at-risk women: Psychological influences and implications

    Gynecological Oncology

    (1999)
  • K.A. Rimes et al.

    Prediction of psychological reactions to bone density screening for osteoporosis using a cognitive-behavioral model of anxiety

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2002)
  • H.M. Warwick et al.

    Hypochondriasis

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1990)
  • P. Absetz et al.

    Experience with breast cancer, pre-screening perceived susceptibility, and the psychological impact of screening

    Psycho-Oncology

    (2003)
  • M.A. Andrykowski et al.

    Psychological impact of benign breast biopsy: A longitudinal, comparative study

    Health Psychology

    (2002)
  • W. Atkin et al.

    Design of a multicentre randomised trial to evaluate flexible sigmoidoscopy in colorectal cancer screening

    Journal of Medical Screening

    (2001)
  • A.J. Barsky et al.

    Resource utilization of patients with hypochondriacal health anxiety and somatization

    Medical Care

    (2001)
  • J. Brett et al.

    Women who are recalled for further investigation for breast screening: Psychological consequences 3 years after recall and factors affecting re-attendance

    Journal of Public Health Medicine

    (2001)
  • C.S. Carver et al.

    On the self-regulation of behavior

    (1998)
  • S. Clutton et al.

    Predictors of emotional well-being following a ‘false-positive’ breast cancer screening result

    Psychology and Health

    (1999)
  • P. Edwards et al.

    Increasing response rates to postal questionnaires: Systematic review

    British Medical Journal

    (2002)
  • R. Ellman et al.

    Psychiatric morbidity associated with screening for breast cancer

    British Journal of Cancer

    (1989)
  • I.T. Gram et al.

    Quality of life following a false positive mammogram

    British Journal of Cancer

    (1990)
  • Cited by (27)

    • Evaluating the Impact of Acromegaly on Quality of Life

      2022, Endocrinology and Metabolism Clinics of North America
      Citation Excerpt :

      Finally, it should be borne in mind that the increased number of diagnoses of cancer could happen in these patients because they are examined more accurately and more frequently before diagnosis (ie, surveillance bias). As colonoscopy screening for colonic neoplasia is recommended every 10 years and more frequently in those with persistently elevated IGF-1, having polyps at previous colonoscopies, or in case of a positive family history of colon cancer,4 it should be kept in mind that screening methods for early cancer detection might lead to significant psychological distress in patients.31,32 Cardiovascular events are responsible for increased mortality rates in acromegaly; however, those with well-controlled acromegaly are now closely approximating that of the general population.27,28

    • Health anxiety in the digital age: An exploration of psychological determinants of online health information seeking

      2015, Computers in Human Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      Those who suffer from elevated levels of health anxiety utilize medical services at a greater frequency and incur higher medical costs than those who do not (Barsky, Ettner, Horsky, & Bates, 2001). These individuals also experience elevated levels of concern when obtaining routine medical care (Miles & Wardle, 2006). Given these findings, it is possible that feelings of health anxiety may also influence one’s inclination to seek out health information online.

    • The link between women's body image disturbances and body-focused cancer screening behaviors: A critical review of the literature and a new integrated model for women

      2013, Body Image
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, one study revealed that a specific domain of cancer-specific health anxiety, intrusive thoughts about cancer, predicted over-performance of breast self-examinations (i.e., more than once a month) among women (Erblich et al., 2000). While regular health screenings typically reduce anxiety for individuals with health anxiety, the over-performance of breast self-examinations appears to exacerbate cancer-specific health anxiety among women (Erblich et al., 2000; Miles & Wardle, 2006). Thus, there appears to be a linear relationship between health anxiety and cancer screening behaviors with increasing amounts of cancer worry predicting more frequent, even excessive, cancer screening behaviors, with no evidence that high levels of cancer worry inhibit screenings (Hay et al., 2006).

    • Screenee perception and health-related quality of life in colorectal cancer screening: A review

      2011, Value in Health
      Citation Excerpt :

      The score for both the questionnaires was lower in the group of subjects attending the screening, indicating a lowered level of psychiatric distress. Miles and Wardle analyzed the psychological distress arising from attending flexible sigmoidoscopy screening in the United Kingdom [72]. A shortened form of the Health Anxiety Questionnaire (HAQ) [73], the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) [74], and the Psychological Consequences of Screening Questionnaire (PCQ) [75] were used.

    • Attitudes to participation in a lung cancer screening trial: A qualitative study

      2012, Thorax
      Citation Excerpt :

      Breast and colon screening studies suggest that ‘cancer-related worry is often associated with perceived risk’.22 In the literature on cancer screening there is debate as to whether worry leads to or deters people from cancer screening.23–25 However, our findings suggest that anxiety deterred participation in screening.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text