Early detection of cancer: knowledge and behavior among Dutch adults

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0361-090X(02)00121-6Get rights and content

Abstract

This paper reports on knowledge and behavior among Dutch adults (n=1530) regarding early detection of cancer (passive detection, active detection and help-seeking behavior). Written questionnaires assessed knowledge of detection methods and cancer symptoms, symptom recognition, detection behaviors, and help-seeking behaviors. Respondents did not have much general knowledge of detection methods and cancer symptoms, but recognized most cancer symptoms. More women than men paid attention to cancer symptoms. For most symptoms, medical help would be sought within the appropriate time by the majority of the respondents. Principal component analyses revealed relations between symptoms pertaining to the ‘interior’ body and those pertaining to the ‘exterior’ body. Correlations between symptom recognition, detection behavior and help-seeking behavior were low. Differences in demographic groups and differences in knowledge and behavior require adaptation of future health education on early detection of cancer to different groups.

Introduction

Detecting cancer in the early stages contributes to improved chances for successful treatment and thus for survival [1], [2], [3], [4]. Three categories of early detection behavior can be distinguished: (1) passive detection behavior, i.e. a person becomes aware of a cancer symptom without a conscious action; (2) active detection behavior, i.e. a conscious action to detect cancer symptoms, such as breast self-examination (BSE); (3) detection behavior by medical professionals, for instance, during a screening program or a medical check-up. Early detection of cancer is not one particular behavior, but consists of several separate behaviors. A person has to be able to recognize the different symptoms, and to know how to examine his or her body. In addition to these behaviors, a person has to visit a health care provider when a symptom is found or to participate in screening programs. Although some of these behaviors overlap, the different behaviors require different knowledge, attitudes and skills.

Most studies of early detection of cancer have focused on active detection behaviors, such as performing breast, testicular and skin self-examination, or on detection by medical health care providers, such as participating in screening programs and having yearly check-ups by a physician [5], [6], [7], [8], [9], [10], [11]. While active detection contributes to early detection of cancer, a significant contribution can also be made by passive detection and by seeking medical help promptly when potential cancer symptoms are discovered. Qin et al. [12] classify cancers in three types, to make clear which cancers can be self-detected: (1) cancers of the superficial organs, such as skin, breast and testis, which can be detected by looking for lumps, ulcerations or moles or by palpation; (2) cancers of the hollow organs, such as urinary bladder, lungs, etc. which can be detected by being alert on, for instance, blood loss; (3) cancer of deep, solid organs, which does not give any signals in the early development of the cancer and which cannot be detected by palpation.

Few studies have focused on the performance of passive detection behaviors. Knowledge of cancer symptoms is a prerequisite for correct interpretation of symptoms and is associated with help-seeking behavior [13], [14], [15], [16]. One study found that Dutch adults knew little about cancer symptoms and detection methods [17]. Moreover, the Dutch study showed there were differences regarding knowledge of cancer symptoms, knowledge of detection methods and passive detection behavior between sexes, age groups, and educational level groups. Compared to men, women had greater knowledge of cancer symptoms and detection methods, and checked themselves more often for cancer symptoms. Knowledge of cancer symptoms can be measured by asking open-ended questions about what cancer symptoms are, or in retrospective studies by asking with closed-ended questions whether the subjects had interpreted symptoms they had experienced as cancer symptoms. A limitation of the first method is that it is not always necessary to be able to actively recall symptoms, since recognition might be sufficient for associating symptoms with a serious health problem and thus seeking help. The second method might be limited by social desirability, resulting in an over-estimation of knowledge.

The aim of the present paper is to assess to what extent Dutch adults recognized cancer symptoms, participated in detection methods (passive detection) and sought medical help for possible cancer symptoms. This study furthermore aimed at determining differences in knowledge and behavior in relation to demographic variables. Furthermore, we analyzed the underlying factors linking cancer symptoms in order to indicate different components related to recognizing symptoms, passive detection and help-seeking behavior, and to assess the relationship between symptom recognition, passive detection and help-seeking.

Section snippets

Respondents and procedure

Data was obtained from 1855 Dutch adults by means of written questionnaires in 1999. Participants were recruited by local newspapers and a national newspaper. Participation was on a voluntary basis. A total of 1778 questionnaires (96%) were completed and returned.

Respondents were predominantly female (80%). The mean age was 46 years (S.D.=13). Most respondents had a spouse or partner (76%), while 24% were single or widowed. Of the respondents 35% had low education, 36% had medium education and

Recognition of cancer symptoms

Table 1 lists the percentages of symptoms correctly recognized as cancer or non-cancer symptoms. The appearance of new moles or warts was the most well-known cancer symptom (79%). Other symptoms were less often mentioned correctly as cancer symptoms (Table 1). Most of the non-cancer symptoms were linked to cancer by fewer than 40% of the respondents, except dizziness, which was associated with cancer by 59% of the respondents.

Differences in total knowledge scores on correctly recognized

Discussion

This study was performed to investigate what the Dutch adult population knew about cancer symptoms, and in what early detection behaviors they actually engaged. The results showed that levels of symptom recognition was moderate for most cancer symptoms and that most non-cancer symptoms were recognized correctly as such. Moreover, respondents reported to be fairly attentive to possible cancer symptoms, although attention was higher for some symptoms than for others. Women were more engaged in

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by a grant from the Dutch Cancer Society.

References (34)

  • L Lechner et al.

    Testicular self-examination (TSE) among Dutch young men aged 15–19: determinants of the intention to practice TSE

    Health Educ. Res.

    (2002)
  • J Wardle et al.

    Testicular self-examination: attitudes and practices among young men in Europe

    Eur. J. Cancer Prev.

    (1994)
  • J Wardle et al.

    Breast self-examination: attitudes and practices among young women in Europe

    Eur. J. Cancer Prev.

    (1995)
  • D.X Qin et al.

    New concept for cancer screening

    Eur. J. Cancer Prev.

    (1996)
  • S.D Cochran et al.

    Correlates in delay of seeking treatment for endometrial cancer

    J. Psychosom. Obstet. Gynaecol.

    (1986)
  • V Mor et al.

    Pre-diagnostic symptom recognition and help seeking among cancer patients

    J. Commun. Health

    (1990)
  • I Sheik et al.

    The role of knowledge and beliefs in help seeking behaviour for cancer: a quantitative and qualitative approach

    Patient Educ. Couns.

    (1998)
  • Cited by (44)

    • Determinants of adherence to screening by colonoscopy in individuals with a family history of colorectal cancer

      2013, Patient Education and Counseling
      Citation Excerpt :

      The fourth and final model was composed of all the variables in the previous models and intention. Previous studies have demonstrated that having alarm symptoms is a catalyst for seeking medical diagnosis and participation in CRC screening [36,47,49,56,57] thus, symptomatic screeners were excluded from the final logistic regression. All statistical analyses were carried out using SAS 9.1.3 (SAS Institute Inc., 2000–2004) statistical programs.

    • Shedding light on women's help seeking behaviour for self discovered breast symptoms

      2013, European Journal of Oncology Nursing
      Citation Excerpt :

      A review of the theoretical literature on help seeking for breast symptoms identified various theories and models used in research with women who are symptom free to determine their intentions to seek help in the event of breast symptom discovery (Facione et al., 2002; Hunter et al., 2003; Burgess et al., 2009). However, it was emphasised that intentions to seek help are not always indicative of what happens in the real life situation of symptom discovery (de Nooijer et al., 2002). The Judgement to Delay Model (Facione et al., 2002) identified the factors (demographics, symptom appraisal, knowledge and beliefs, health related habits, health service system issues, affective responses, relationship constraints and personality attributes) hypothesised to influence women's HSB should they discover a breast symptom.

    • Women's help seeking behaviour for self discovered breast cancer symptoms

      2011, European Journal of Oncology Nursing
      Citation Excerpt :

      This model presents the variables that impact on women’s intentions to seek help for hypothetical breast symptoms. It is asserted that “intention is generally one of the most significant predictors of behaviour” (deNooijer et al., 2002 p.368). However, it is also reiterated that while actual help seeking may be expected in certain situations, it is not certain that it will occur in reality.

    • How would early detection be possible? An enquiry into cancer related knowledge, understanding and health seeking behaviour of urban black women in Tshwane, South Africa

      2010, European Journal of Oncology Nursing
      Citation Excerpt :

      It is therefore doubtful if the woman, except for the possibility of a lump in the breast, would recognize any sign of cancer and consider it to be serious. To worsen the situation, De Nooijer et al. (2002) found that even if a person are aware of the signs of cancer, it would not automatically imply that he or she would be aware of cancer or seek healthcare should such a sign become evident. Not being able to recognize the importance or seriousness of the symptom also contributes to delayed health seeking behaviour (Corner et al., 2006).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text