Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Immediate hedonic response to smoking lapses: relationship to smoking relapse, and effects of nicotine replacement therapy

  • Original Investigation
  • Published:
Psychopharmacology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Objective and rationale

Smoking lapses represent an important juncture between smoking cessation and relapse. Nicotine replacement therapy (NRT) has been shown to decrease the risk of progression from lapse to relapse. We hypothesized that this effect might be mediated via decreases in reinforcement from smoking lapses.

Method

We assessed 169 subjects who lapsed during treatment in a double-blind placebo-controlled study of high-dose (35 mg) nicotine patch. Following their first lapse, using an electronic diary, subjects recorded the amount they smoked, and rated the pleasantness and satisfaction (“hedonic rating”) and the aversiveness of smoking. Subjects were then followed and assessed for further lapses and relapses.

Results

Subjects who smoked more during the first lapse had greater risk of progression [second lapse: hazard ratio (HR)=1.16, confidence interval (CI)=1.01–1.32; relapse: HR=1.22, CI=0.97–1.54]. Subjects with higher hedonic ratings of the first lapse also had a greater risk of progression to the second lapse (HR=1.08, CI=1.02–1.14) and to relapse (HR=1.26, CI=1.11–1.41). Aversive ratings had no bearing on progression. As expected, active treatment reduced the risk of both a second lapse (HR=0.54, CI=0.39–0.78) and a relapse (HR=0.22, CI=0.11–0.45). Importantly, however, NRT had no effect on hedonic ratings, amount smoked during the first lapse, or aversive ratings.

Conclusions

Hedonic response to an initial lapse predicted progression to relapse, but this did not mediate the effect of NRT on progression.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Because many second lapses (27.8%) occurred on the same day as the first lapse, we also analyzed time to second lapse using hours as the unit of analysis. As the results were largely the same, we have not presented these analyses.

  2. As less than half of the sample reached the relapse end point, the median value presented underestimated the true median for all subjects who would eventually relapse.

  3. When considering the comparison between baseline ratings and lapse hedonic ratings, it should be noted that the two “assessments” were not strictly identical. Firstly, the smoking “situation” was different—whereas lapse assessments were likely colored by feelings of guilt and disappointment, it was unlikely that such feelings were present during baseline smoking. Additionally, baseline assessment could have been performed up to 30 min after finishing the cigarette being rated and, hence, was subjected to a greater degree of memory “bias.” Nevertheless, we feel that the comparison was still informative.

  4. In addition, the effect of patch, as noted in Levin et al. (1994), was found in hedonic ratings on weeks 2 and 3 of treatment, not during the first week of treatment (when most of our first-lapse episodes were obtained). To test whether our findings might be affected by differential reactions to early (week 1) vs later lapses, we divided our sample into lapses in week 1 and later. There was no indication of an effect of time: the effects were very nearly identical and the time × treatment interaction was nonsignificant (p>0.90).

References

  • Baron RM, Kenny DA (1986) The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 51:1173–1182

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Berridge KC, Robinson TE (1998) What is the role of dopamine in reward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brains Res Rev 28:309–369

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Berridge KC, Robinson TE (2003) Parsing reward. Trends Neurosci 26:507–513

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Borland R (1990) Slip-ups and relapse in attempts to quit smoking. Addict Behav 15:235–245

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brandon TH, Tiffany ST, Obremski KM, Baker TB (1990) Postcessation cigarette use: the process of relapse. Addict Behav 15:105–114

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brownell KD, Marlatt GA, Lichtenstein E, Wilson GT (1986) Understanding and preventing relapse. Am Psychol 41:765–782

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chornock WM, Stitzer ML, Gross J, Leischow S (1992) Experimental model of smoking re-exposure: effects on relapse. Psychopharmacology 108:495–500

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd edn. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • deWit H, Stewart J (1981) Reinstatement of cocaine-reinforced responding in the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 75:134–143

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • deWit H, Stewart J (1983) Drug reinstatement of heroin reinforced responding in the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 79:29–31

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Eissenberg T, Balster RL (2000) Initial tobacco use episodes in children and adolescents: current knowledge, future directions. Drug Alcohol Depend 59:41–60

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fant RV, Schuh KJ, Stitizer ML (1995) Response to smoking as a function of prior smoking amounts. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 119:385–390

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Foulds J, Stapleton J, Feyerabend C, Vesey C, Jarvis M, Russell MAH (1992) Effect of transdermal nicotine patches on cigarette smoking: a double blind crossover study. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 106:421–427

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman LS, Lichtenstein E, Biglan A (1985) Smoking onset among teens, an empirical analysis of initial situations. Addict Behav 10:1–13

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Glautier S (2004) Measures and models of nicotine dependence: positive reinforcement. Addiction 99(Suppl 1):30–50

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hahn G, Charlin VL, Sussman S, Dent CW, Flay B, Hansen WB, Burton D (1990) Adolescents’ first and most recent use situations of smokeless tobacco and cigarettes: similarities and differences. Addict Behav 15:439–448

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hajek P, Stead LF, West R, Jarvis M (2005) Relapse prevention interventions for smoking cessation. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 25:CD003999

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenford SL, Fiore MC, Jorenby DE, Smith SS, Welter D, Baker TB (1994) Predicting smoking cessation: who will quit with and without the nicotine patch. JAMA 271:589–594

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Leri F, Stewart J (2002a) The consequences of different “lapses” on relapse to heroin seeking in rats. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 10:339–349

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Leri F, Stewart J (2002b) Advantages and disadvantages of studying the process of relapse in rats: reply to Baker and Curtin (2002), Bardo (2002), Goeders (2002), Marlatt (2002), and Tiffany and Conklin (2002) Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 10:364–366

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Levin ED, Westman EC, Stein RM, Carnahan E, Sanchez M, Herman S et al (1994) Nicotine skin patch treatment increases abstinence, decreases withdrawal symptoms, and attenuates rewarding effects of smoking. J Clin Psychopharmacol 14:41–49

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lichtenstein E, Glasgow RE (1992) Smoking cessation: what have we learned over the past decade? J Consult Clin Psychol 60:518–527

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Marlatt GA, Gordon JR (1985) Relapse prevention. Guildford Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Mathieu-Kia AM, Kellogg SH, Butelman ER, Kreek MJ (2002) Nicotine addiction: insights from recent animal studies. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 162:102–118

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nil R, Woodson PP, Battig K (1987) Effects of smoking deprivation on smoking behavior and heart rate response in high and low CO absorbing smokers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 92:465–469

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ockene JK, Emmons KM, Mermelstein RJ, Perkins KA, Bonollo DS, Voorhees C et al (2000) Relapse and maintenance issues for smoking cessation. Health Psychol 19:17–31

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ossip-Klein DJ, Bigelow G, Parker SR, Curry S, Hall S, Kirkland S (1986) Task force 1: classification and assessment of smoking behavior. Health Psychol 5:3–11

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perkins KA, DiMarco A, Grobe JE, Scierka A, Stiller RL (1994a) Nicotine discrimination in male and female smokers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 116:407–413

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Perkins KA, Epstein LH, Grobe J, Fonte C (1994b) Tobacco abstinence, smoking cues, and the reinforcing value of smoking. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 47:107–112

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pomerleau OF, Pomerleau CS (1992) Euphoriant effects of nicotine in smokers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 108:460–465

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pomerleau OF, Pomerleau CS, Namenek RJ (1998) Early experiences with tobacco among women smokers, ex-smokers, and never-smokers. Addiction 93:595–599

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson TE, Berridge KC (2001) Incentive-sensitization and addiction. Addiction 96:103–114

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rose JE (1984) Discriminability of nicotine in tobacco smoke: implications for titration. Addict Behav 9:189–193

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rose JE, Westman EC, Behm FM, Johnson MP, Goldberg JS (1999) Blockade of smoking satisfaction using the peripheral nicotinic antagonist trimethaphan. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 62:165–172

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shaham Y, Shalev U, Lu L, deWit H, Stewart J (2003) The reinstatement model of drug relapse: history, methodology and major findings. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 168:3–20

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shiffman S (1982) Relapse following smoking cessation: a situational analysis. J Consult Clin Psychol 50:71–86

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shiffman S, Hickcox M, Paty JA, Gnys M, Kassel JD, Richards TJ (1996a) Progression from a smoking lapse to relapse: prediction from abstinence violation effects, nicotine dependence, and lapse characteristics. J Consult Clin Psychol 64:993–1002

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shiffman S, Paty JA, Gnys M, Kassel JD, Hickcox M (1996b) First lapses to smoking: within subjects analysis of real time reports. J Consult Clin Psychol 64:366–379

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shiffman S, Ferguson SG, Gwaltney C, Balabanis M, Shadel W (in press-a) Reduction of abstinence induced withdrawal and craving using nicotine replacement therapy. Psychopharmacology

  • Shiffman S, Scharf D, Shadel W, Gwaltney C, Dang Q, Paton S, Clark D (in press-b) Analyzing milestones in smoking cessation: an illustration from a randomized trial of high dose nicotine patch. J Consult Clin Psychol

  • Stewart J, de Wit H, Eikelboom R (1984) Role of unconditioned and conditioned drug effects in the self-administration of opiates and stimulants. Psychol Rev 91:251–268

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stone AA, Shiffman S (1994) Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) in behavioral medicine. Ann Behav Med 16:199–202

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutton SR (1989) Relapse following smoking cessation: a critical review of current theory and research. In: Gossop M (ed) Relapse and addiction behaviour. Routledge, London, pp 41–72

    Google Scholar 

  • Tiffany ST (1990) A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. Psychol Rev 97:147–168

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Waters AJ, Shiffman S, Bradley BP, Mogg K (2003a) Attentional shifts to smoking cues in smokers. Addiction 98:1409–1417

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waters AJ, Shiffman S, Sayette MA, Paty JA, Gwaltney CJ, Balabanis MH (2003b) Attentional bias predicts outcome in smoking cessation. Health Psychol 22:378–387

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waters AJ, Shiffman S, Sayette MA, Paty JA, Gwaltney CJ, Balabanis MH (2004) Cue provoked craving and nicotine replacement therapy in smoking cessation. J Consult Clin Psychol 72:1136–1142

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wise RA (1996) Addictive drugs and brain stimulation reward. Annu Rev Neurosci 19:319–340

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by a grant (DA 06084) from the National Institute on Drug Abuse to Saul Shiffman. We are grateful to GlaxoSmithKline Consumer Healthcare (GSKCH) for providing nicotine and placebo patches for the study (GSKCH did not otherwise participate in the study or in the paper).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Saul Shiffman.

Additional information

Fund declarations

Saul Shiffman and Stuart Ferguson consult exclusively for GlaxoSmithKline Consumer Healthcare on matters relating to existing smoking cessation products. Dr. Shiffman also has an interest in a new smoking cessation product and is a founder of invivodata, which provides electronic diaries for clinical trials.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Shiffman, S., Ferguson, S.G. & Gwaltney, C.J. Immediate hedonic response to smoking lapses: relationship to smoking relapse, and effects of nicotine replacement therapy. Psychopharmacology 184, 608–618 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-005-0175-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-005-0175-4

Keywords

Navigation