Article Text

Download PDFPDF

Thorax at 60
Free
  1. A E Tattersfield
  1. Correspondence to:
    Professor A E Tattersfield
    Division of Respiratory Medicine, Clinical Sciences Building, Nottingham University Hospital, City Hospital Campus, Nottingham NG5 1PB, UK; anne.tattersfield{at}nottingham.ac.uk

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

Editors look for articles that say something new, and studies don’t have to be large and grand. I have selected two papers and an editorial from among the many I could have chosen. They may not be landmark articles, but they are probably the three articles I have quoted most widely in discussions and teaching. I enjoy the fact that the two studies are simple, showing that valuable information can still be obtained by studying patients carefully and without breaking the bank.

First is a paper by Tweedale et al1 which looked at short term repeatability of FEV1 and bronchodilator responsiveness in patients with airflow obstruction. The authors showed that within subject differences in FEV1 measurements made 20 minutes apart were usually within 160 ml, and that this was true whether the initial FEV1 was 1 or 5 litres. This has been enormously useful when planning studies and also when interpreting small changes in FEV1 in individual patients. The article also highlighted the difficulty of defining a bronchodilator response—whether to use a percentage change which favours patients with a low FEV1 or an absolute response exceeding 160 ml, which favours those with a high FEV1—to mention just two methods. The paper didn’t provide the answer but, by relating responses to repeatability, provided a clearer picture of how such data should be interpreted.

Second is an even simpler study,2 carried out I think by a medical student, which refuted the classical teaching that detection of cyanosis implies that the patient has 5 g/dl reduced haemoglobin. This assertion had been questioned by Flenley among others but was (and still is) repeated by most medical students and some widely used textbooks. In the Thorax paper two observers looked for cyanosis in 80 normothermic patients with a wide range of arterial oxygen tensions. Cyanosis was detected invariably when the reduced haemoglobin concentration approached 1.5 g/dl, equivalent to an oxygen saturation of around 90% in patients with a normal haemoglobin concentration. This can be confirmed easily on any ward round. Although knowing the correct cut off may be less important now that oximeters are widely available, understanding the physiological basis of clinical signs is important for sensible interpretation and management. A further point is whether students will be penalised if they give the correct answer, since many teachers and presumably examiners still appear to believe that the cut off is 5 g/dl. If they are, please refer the examiner to this paper.

And finally—the editorial3 in which McNeil, Sveger and Thelin discussed the psychosocial effects of neonatal screening for somatic mutations, based on screening for α1-antitrypsin deficiency in Sweden in the 1970s. It may seem obvious that parents who were told that their child had α1-antitrypsin deficiency would take extra precautions to ensure that the child was brought up in a smoke free environment. Not so, however. Follow up studies showed that the stress induced by receiving information about a “potential disease” had the opposite effect, with at least one parent smoking in over half the homes (and twice as many fathers smoked compared with a control group) and the children showed more behavioural problems. The 1970s were early days for understanding the natural history of α1-antitrypsin deficiency, for genetic counselling, and for the public’s understanding of genetics, and all would be very different today. The message, however, that the effects of counselling need to be tested and cannot necessarily be assumed, is still valid.

Editor, 1987–1991

REFERENCES

View Abstract